Vigilance as key cognitive function in the relationship between acute physical exercise and cognition

Authors

  • Francisco Tomás González-Fernández Universidad Internacional de Valencia Spain

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24310/riccafd.2019.v8i1.5762

Keywords:

sustained attention, acute exercise, cognition

Abstract

The last two decades, there have been numerous studies that have shown the beneficial effects of physical activity on health (Biddle, Fox y Boutcher, 2003; Lee y Skerritt, 2001). Far from believing that physical activity only prevented physical disorders [e.g., cardiovascular disease (Haskell et al., 2007)], the scientific literature argues that it also prevents mental disorders [e.g., depression and stress reduction (Dunn et al., 2001) or anxiety (Paluska y Schwenk, 2000)]. In addition, organizations of global relevance such as World Health Organization (WHO), Centers for disease Control and Prevention (CDC), American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) or the American Heart Association (AHA), have positively related physical activity to general health and contemplate physical activity as essential and indispensable to preserve health. The present work aimed to investigate the effects of acute exercise on cognitive functioning (vigilance and perception) and suggest that moderate-light effort increment general activation and improve the speed of response in sustained attention tasks independently of target stimulus relevance.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

Author Biography

Francisco Tomás González-Fernández, Universidad Internacional de Valencia

Doctor Internacional en Biomedicina. Profesor e Investigador. Universidad Internacional de Valencia

References

Aks, D. J. (1998). Influence of exercise on visual search: Implications for mediating cognitive mechanisms. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 771–783.

Arent, S. M., y Landers, D. M. (2003). Arousal, anxiety, and performance: A reexamination of the inverted-u hypothesis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74(4), 436-444.

Arcelin, R., Brisswalter, J., y Delignieres, D. (1997). Effects of physical exercise duration on decision making performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 32, 123-140.

Arcelin, R., Delignieres, D., y Brisswalter, J. (1998). Selective effects of physical exercise on choice reaction processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 175-185.

Ando, S., Kimura T., Hamada, T., Kokubu, M., Moritani, T., y Oda, S. (2005). Increase in reaction time for the peripheral visual field during exercise above the ventilaroty threshold. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 94: 461-467.

Audiffren, M., Tomporowski, P. D., y Zagrodnik, J. (2008). Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: Energizing motor processes during a choice reaction time task. Acta Psychologica, 129, 410-419.

Audiffren, M., Tomporowski, P. D., y Zagrodnik, J. (2009). Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: Modulation of executive control in a Random Number Generation task. Acta Psychologica, 132, 85-95.

Bard, C., y Fleury, M. (1978). Influence of imposed metabolic fatigue on visual capacitycomponents. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 1283-1287.

Ballard, J.C. (1996). Computerized assessment of sustained attention: a review of factors affecting vigilance performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18 (6), 843-863.

Bailey, S. P., Holt, C., Pfluger, K. C., La Budde, Z., Afergan, D., Stripling, R., Miller, P. C., y Hall, E. E. (2008). Impact of prolonged exercise in the heat and carbohydrate supplementation on performance of a virtual environment task. Military Medicine, 173(2), 187–192.

Basner M., y Dinges D.F., (2011). Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep, 34(5), 581-591.

Brisswalter, J., Arcelin, R., Audiffren, M., y Delignières, D. (1997). Influence of physical exercise on simple reaction time: effect of physical fitness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 1019-1027.

Brisswalter, J., Collardeau, M., y Arcelin, R. (2002). Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Medicine, 32(9), 555-566.

Brisswalter, J., Collardeau, M., y Rene, A. (2002). Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Medicine, 32, 555–566.

Brisswalter, J., Durand, M., Delignieres, D., y Legros, P. (1995). Optimal and non-optimal demand in a dual-task of pedaling and simple reaction time: Effects on energy expenditure and cognitive performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 29, 15-34.

Caldwell, J.A., Caldwell, J.L., Smith, J.K., y Brown, D.L. (2004) Modafinil's effects on simulator performance and mood in pilots during 37 h without sleep. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 75(9):777-84.

Chang, Y.K., Labban, J.D., Gapin, J.I., y Etnier J.L. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Research, 1453, 87-101.

Chmura, J., Nazar, K., y Kaciuba-Uscilko, H. (1994). Choice reaction time during graded exercise in relation to blood lactate and plasma catecholamine thresholds. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 15, 172-176.

Chmura, J., Krysztofiak, H., Ziemba, A.W. (1998). Psychomotor performance during prolonged exercise above and below the blood lactate threshold. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 77, 77-80

Cian, C., Barraud, P. A., Melin, B., y Raphel, C. (2001). Effects of fluid ingestion on cognitive function after heat stress or exercise-induced dehydration. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 243-251.

Cian, C., Koulmannn, N., Barraud, P. A., Raphel, C., Jimenez, C., y Melin, B. (2000). Influences of variations in body hydration on cognitive function: Effects of hyperhydration, heat stress, and exercise-induced dehydration. Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 29-36.

Clark, C.R., Geffen, G.M., y Geffen, L.B. (1989). Catecholamines and the covert orientation of attention in humans. Neuropsychologia, 27 (2):131-9.

Correa, A., Molina, E., Sanabria, D. (2014). Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. Accident Analysis y Prevention, 67, 113-118.

Collardeau, M., Brisswalter, J., y Audiffren, M. (2001). Effects of a prolonged run on simple reaction time of well trained runners. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 679-689.

Collardeau, M., Brisswalter, J., Vercruyssen, F., Audiffren, M. y Goubault, C. (2001). Single and choice reaction time during prolonged exercise in trained subjects: Influence of carbohydrate availability. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 86, 150-156.

Cote, J., Salmela, J., y Papathanasopoulu, K. P. (1992). Effects of progressive exercise on attentional focus. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 351-354.

Culham, J. C., Cavanagh, P., y Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Attention response functions: characterizing brain areas using fMRI activation during parametric variations of attentional load. Neuron, 32(4), 737-745.

Davranche, K., y Audiffren M. (2004). Facilitating effects of exercise on information processing. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 419–428.

Davranche, K., Burle, B., Audiffren, M., y Hasbroucq, T. (2005). Information processing during physical exercise: A chronometric and electromyographic study. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 532-540.

Davranche, K., Burle, B., Audiffren, M., y Hasbroucq, T. (2006). Physical exercise facilitates motor processes in simple reaction time performance: An electromyographic analysis. Neuroscience Letters, 396(1), 54-56.

Delignieres, D., Brisswalter, J., y Legros, P. (1994). Influence of physical exercise on choice reaction time in sports experts: The mediating role of resource allocation. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 27, 173-188.

Del Giorno, J. M., Hall, E. E., O’Leary, K. C., Bixby, W. R., y Miller, P. C. (2010). Cognitive function during acute exercise: a test of the transient hypofrontality theory. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 32, 312–323

Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 231–256.

Dietrich, A. (2006). Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiatry Research, 145, 79-83.

Dietrich, A., y Audiffren, M. (2011). The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1305-1325.

Dietrich, A., y Sparling, P. B. (2004). Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal-dependent cognition. Brain and Cognition, 55, 516-524.

Dorrian, J., Rogers, N.L., y Dinges, D.F. (2005). Psychomotor vigilance performance: Neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In Kushida, C. A. (Ed.), Sleep Deprivation. Clinical issues, pharmacology, and sleep loss effects (39-70). New York, NY: Marcel Dekker.

Dorrian, J., Roach, G.D., Fletcher, A., y Dawson, D. (2007). Simulated train driving: fatigue, self-awareness and cognitive disengagement. Applied ergonomics, 38 (2): 155-66.

Drummond, S.P., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Dinges, D., Ayalon, L., Mednick, S.C., y Meloy, M.J. (2005). The Neural Basis of the Psychomotor Vigilance Task. Sleep. 28 (9), 1059-1068.

Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 1959(66), 183-201.

Etnier, J. L., Nowell, P. M., Landers, D. M., y Sibley, B. A. (2006). A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Research Review, 52, 119-130.

Etnier, J. L., Salazar, W., Landers, D. M., Petruzzello, S. J., Han, M., y Nowell, P. (1997). The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 249-277.

Fery, Y. A., Ferry, A., Vom-Hofe, A., y Rieu, M. (1997). Effect of physical exhaustion on cognitive functioning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 291-198.

Fleury, M., y Brad, C. (1987). Effects of different types of physical activity on the performance of perceptual tasks in peripheral and central vision and coincident timing. Ergonomics, 30, 945-958.

Gondola, J. C. (1987). The effects of a single bout of aerobic dancing on selected tests of creativity. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 275-278.

Hancock, P.A., y Hart, S.G. (2002). Defeating terrorism: What can human factors/ergonomics offer? Ergonomics in Design, 10 (1), 6-16.

Heckler, B., y Croce, R. (1992). Effects of time of posttest after two durations of exercise on speed and accuracy of addition and subtraction by fit and less-fit women. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1059-1065.

Herrmann, D. J., Yoder, C., Gruneberg, M., y Payne, D. (2006). Applied cognitive psychology: A textbook (1st ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Castelli, D. M., Hall, E. E., y Kramer, A. F. (2009). The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience, 159, 1044-1054.

Hillman, C. H., Snook, E. M., y Jerome, G. J. (2003). Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48, 307-314.

Hockey, G. R. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: a cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 73–93.

Hockey, G.R., Gaillard, A.W., y Coles, M.G. (1986). Energetic Aspects of Human Information Processing. Nijhoff, Netherlands.

Hogervorst, E., Riedel, W., Jeukendrup, A., y Jolles, J. (1996). Cognitive performance after strenuous physical exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 479-488.

Humphreys, M. S., y Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review, 91(2), 153-184.

Ide, K., Schmalbruch, I. K., Quistorff, B., Horn, A., y Secher, N. H. (2000). Lactate, glucose and O2 uptake in human brain during recovery from maximal exercise. Journal of Physiology, 522, 159-164.

Isaacs, L. D., y Pohlman, E. L. (1991). Effects of exercise intensity on an accompanying timing task. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 20, 123-131.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

Kamijo, K., Hayashi, Y., Sakai, T., Yahiro, T., Tanaka, K., y Nishihira, Y. (2009). Acute effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in older adults. Journal of Gerontology B-Psychology, 64, 356–363.

Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Hatta, A., Kaneda, T., Kida, T., Higashiura, T., et al. (2004). Changes in arousal level by differential exercise intensity. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 2693-2698.

Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Hatta, A., Kaneda, T., Wasaka, T., Kida, T., y Kuroiwa, K. (2004). Differential influences of exercise intensity on information processing in the central nervous system. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 92, 305-311.

Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Higashiura, T., y Kuroiwa K. (2007). The interactive effect of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing. International Journal of Psychophysioly, 65(2):114-21.

Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M.B., O’Leary, K.C., Scudder, M.R., Wu, C.T., Castelli, D.M., y Hillman, C.H. (2011). The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Developmental Science. 14(5):1046-58.

Kruk, B., Chmura, J., Krzeminski, K., Ziemba, A.W., Nazar, K., Pekkarinen, H., y Kaciuba-Uscilko, H. (2001). Influence of caffeine, cold and exercise on multiple choice reaction time. Psychopharmacology, 157, 197–201.

Kubitz, K.A., y Pothakos, K., (1997). Does aerobic exercise decrease brain activation? Journal of Sport y Psychology. 19, 291-301.

Lambourne K., y Tomporowski PD. (2010). The effect of acute exercise on cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 1341, 12–24.

Langner, R., y Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining Attention to Simple Tasks: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Neural Mechanisms of Vigilant Attention. Psychological bulletin, 139(4), 870-900.

Levitt, S., y Gutin, B. (1971). Multiple choice reaction time and movement time during physical exertion. Research Quarterly, 42, 405-411.

Llorens, F., Sanabria, D., y Huertas, F. (2015). The influence of acute intense exercise on exogenous spatial attention depends on physical fitness level. Experimental Psychology, 62 (1): 20-9.

Luque-Casado, A., Perakakis, P., Ciria, L., y Sanabria, D. (2016a). Transient autonomic responses during sustained attention in high and low fit young adults. Scientific Reports. 6: 27556.

Luque-Casado, A., Perakakis, P., Hillman, C. H., Kao, S.C., Llorens, F., Guerra, P., y Sanabria, D. (2016b). Differences in Sustained Attention Capacity as a Function of Aerobic Fitness. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 48 (5), 887–895.

Magnié, M.N., Bermon, S., Martin, F., Madany-Lounis, M., Suisse, G., Muhammad, W., y Dolisi, C. (2000). P300, N400, aerobic fitness, and maximal aerobic exercise. Psychophysiology, 37(3):369-77.

Manly, T., Owen, A.M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G.H., Scott, S.K., y Robertson, I.H. (2003). Enhancing the sensitivity of a sustained attention task to frontal damage: convergent clinical and functional imaging evidence. Neurocase, 9:340–349.

Martens, R., y Landers, D. M. (1970). Motor performance under stress: A test of the inverted-u hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 29-37.

Mackworth, J.F., y Webb, E.C. (1948). The inhibition of serum cholinesterase by alkyl fluorophosphonates. Biomechenical Journal, 42 (1): 91-5.

McGlynn, G. H., Laughlin, N. T., y Bender, V. I. (1977). Effect of strenuous to exhaustive exercise on a discrimination task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 1139-1147.

McGlynn, G. H., Laughlin, N. T., y Vivienne, R. (1979). The effect of increasing levels of exercise on mental performance. Ergonomics, 22, 407-414.

McMorris, T. (1995). The effect of exercise on simple reaction time. In R. Vanfraechem-Raway y Y. Vanden Auweele (Eds.), Proceedings of the IXth European congress on sport psychology (pp. 952-959). Brussels: Belgian Federation of Sport Psychology.

McMorris, T., Collard, K., Corbett, J., Dicks, M., y Swain, J. P. (2008). A test of the catecholamines hypothesis for an acute exercise–cognition interaction. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 89, 106-115.

McMorris, T., Davranche, K., Jones, G., Hall, B., Corbett, J., y Minter, C. (2009). Acute incremental exercise, performance of a central executive task, and sympathoadrenal system and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73(3), 334-340.

McMorris, T., Delves, S., Sproule, J., Lauder, M., y Hale, B. (2005). Effect of incremental exercise on initiation and movement times in a choice response, whole body psychomotor task. British Journal of Sport Medicine, 39(8):537-41.

McMorris, T., y Graydon, J. (1996a). Effects of exercise on soccer decision-making tasks of differing complexities. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 30, 177-193.

McMorris, T., y Graydon, J. (1996b). The effects of exercise on the decision-making performance of experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 109-114.

McMorris, T., y Graydon, J. (1997). Effect of exercise on cognitive performance in soccer-specific tests. Journal of Sports Sciences, 15, 459-468.

McMorris, T., y Graydon, J. (2000). The effect of incremental exercise on cognitive performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 31, 66-81.

McMorris, T., y Hale, B.J. (2012). Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation. Brain Cognition, 80 (3), 338–351.

McMorris, T., y Hale, B.J. (2015). Is there an acute exercise-induced physiological/biochemical threshold which triggers increased speed of cognitive functioning? A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 4, 4-13.

McMorris, T., y Keen, P. (1994). Effect of exercise on simple reaction times of recreational athletes. Percepual and Motor Skills, 78, 123-130.

McMorris, T., Tallon, M., Williams, C., Sproule, J., Potter, J., Swain, J., et al. (2003). Incremental exercise, plasma concentrations of catecholamines, reaction time, and motor time during performance of a noncompatible choice response time task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 590–604.

McMorris, T., Tomporowski, P., y Audiffren, M. (2009). Exercise and cognitive function. Michigan: Wiley-Blackwell.

Muto, V., Jaspar, M., Meyer, C., Kussé, C., Chellappa, S. L., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Shaffii-Le Bourdiec, A., Luxen, A., Middleton, B., Archer, S.N., Phillips, C., Collette, F., Vandewalle, G., Dijk, D-J., y Maquet, P. (2016). Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science, 353(6300), 687-690.

Oken, B. S., Salinsky, M. C., y Elsas, S. M. (2006). Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(9), 1885-1901.

Paas, F.G y Adam, J.J. (1991). Human information processing during physical exercise. Ergonomics, 34, 1385-1397.

Pennekamp, P., Boesel, R., Mecklinger, A., y Ott, H. (1994). Differences in EEG-theta for responded and omitted targets in a sustained attention task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 8, 131-141.

Pesce, C., Capranica, L., Tesittore, A., y Figura, F. (2002). Effects of a submaximal physical load on the orienting and focusing of visual attention. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 42, 401-420.

Petersen, S. E., y Posner, M. I. (2012). The Attention System of the Human Brain: 20 Years After. Annual review of neuroscience, 35, 73-89.

Petruzzello, S.J., Landers, D.M., Hatfield, B.D., Kubitz, K.A., y Salazar, W. (1991). A meta-analysis on the anxiety-reducing effects of acute and chronic exercise. Outcomes and mechanisms. Sports Medicine, 11(3):143-82.

Pontifex, M.B., y Hillman, C.H. (2007). Neuroelectric and behavioral indices of interference control during acute cycling. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(3):570-80.

Pontifex, M.B., Saliba, B.J., Raine, L.B., Picchietti, D.L., y Hillman, C.H. (2013). Exercise improves behavioral, neurocognitive, and scholastic performance in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Pediatrics, 162 (3):543-51.

Reilly, T., y Smith, D. (1986). Effect of work intensity on performance in a psychomotor task during exercise. Ergonomics, 29, 601-606.

Rosenberg, M. D., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., Papademetris, X., Shen, X., Constable, R. T., y Chun, M. M. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nature neuroscience, 19(1), 165-171.

Salmela, J. H., y Ndoye, O. D. (1986). Cognitive distortions during progressive exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 1067-1072.

Sanabria, D., Morales, E., Luque, A., Galvez, G., Huertas, F., y Lupiáñez, J. (2011). Effects of aerobic exercise on exogenus spatial attention. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12(5), 570-574.

Sarter, M., Given, B., y Bruno, J.P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Reviews, 35(2):146-160.

Scudder, M.R., Drollette, E.S., Pontifex, M.B., y Hillman, C.H. (2012). Neuroelectric indices of goal maintenance following a single bout of physical activity. Biological Psychology, 89 (2): 528-31.

Sibley, B. A., Etnier, J. L., y Le Masurier, G. C. (2006). Effects of an acute bout of exercise on cognitive aspects of Stroop performance. Journal of Sport y Exercise Psychology, 28, 285–299.

Sjoberg, H., (1977). Interaction of task difficulty, activation, and workload. Journal of Human Stress 3, 33–38.

Smit, A. S., Eling, P. A., Hopman, M. T., y Coenen, A. M. (2005). Mental and physical efforts affect vigilance differently. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 211 217.

Smith, P.J., Blumenthal, J.A., Hoffman, B.M., Cooper, H., Strauman, T.A., Welsh-Bohmer, K., Browndyke, J.N., y Sherwood, A. (2010). Aerobic Exercise and Neurocognitive Performance: A Meta-Analytic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(3):239–52.

Tenenbaum, G., Yuval, R., Elbaz, G., Gar-Eli, M., y Weinberg, R. (1993). The relationship between cognitive characteristics and decision making. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology, 18, 48-62.

Tomporowski, P., Cureton, K., Armstrong, L., Kane, G., Sparling, P., y Millard-Stafford, M. (2005). Short-term effect of aerobic exercise on executive processes and emotional reactivity. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3(2), 131–146.

Tomporowski, P. D., Ellis, N. R., y Stephens, R. (1987). The immediate effects of strenuous exercise on free recall memory. Ergonomics, 30, 121-129.

Tomporowski, P. D., Lambourne, K., y Okumura, M. S. (2011). Physical activity interventions and children’s mental function: an introduction and overview. Preventive Medicine, 52 Suppl 1, S3-9.

Travlos, A. K., y Marisi, D. Q. (1995). Information processing and concentration as a function of fitness level and exercise induced activation to exhaustion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80, 15-26.

Warm, J.S., Parasuraman, R., y Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors, 50:433-41.

Wiggins, M.W. (2011). Vigilance decrement during a simulated general aviation flight. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1; 25(2):229–35.

Yagi, Y., Coburn, K.L., Estes, K.M., y Arruda, J.E. (1999). Effects of aerobic exercise and gender on visual and auditory P300, reaction time, and accuracy. European Journal of Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 80 (5): 402-8.

Yerkes, R.M. y Dodson, J.D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459–482.

Published

2019-03-31

How to Cite

González-Fernández, F. T. (2019). Vigilance as key cognitive function in the relationship between acute physical exercise and cognition. Revista Iberoamericana De Ciencias De La Actividad Física Y El Deporte, 8(1), 39–53. https://doi.org/10.24310/riccafd.2019.v8i1.5762

Issue

Section

Artículos